Serial Processing Definition

 Posted admin
Serial Processing Definition Average ratng: 3,6/5 5368 votes
  1. Serial Processing Brain
  2. Serial Processing Psychology

Serial memory processing is the act of attending to and processing one item at a time. This is usually contrasted against parallel memory processing, which is the act of attending to and processing all items simultaneously.

A technique for solving problems in which a computer begins at the start and works through to the end in a linear fashion. (Compareparallel processing.)

Nearby words

Serial
serial extraction, serial killer, serial monogamy, serial number, serial port, serial processing, serial radiography, serial rights, serial section, serial-access, serialism
The New Dictionary of Cultural Literacy, Third Edition Copyright © 2005 by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. Published by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. All rights reserved.
Published online 2015 Sep 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01366
PMID: 26441742
This article has been cited by other articles in PMC.

Abstract

In the context of performance optimizations in multitasking, a central debate has unfolded in multitasking research around whether cognitive processes related to different tasks proceed only sequentially (one at a time), or can operate in parallel (simultaneously). This review features a discussion of theoretical considerations and empirical evidence regarding parallel versus serial task processing in multitasking. In addition, we highlight how methodological differences and theoretical conceptions determine the extent to which parallel processing in multitasking can be detected, to guide their employment in future research. Parallel and serial processing of multiple tasks are not mutually exclusive. Therefore, questions focusing exclusively on either task-processing mode are too simplified. We review empirical evidence and demonstrate that shifting between more parallel and more serial task processing critically depends on the conditions under which multiple tasks are performed. We conclude that efficient multitasking is reflected by the ability of individuals to adjust multitasking performance to environmental demands by flexibly shifting between different processing strategies of multiple task-component scheduling.

Keywords: multitasking, dual-tasking, crosstalk, psychological refractory period (PRP), cognitive control, functional bottleneck, bottleneck, parallel versus serial processing

Introduction

A central aim in cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience research on multitasking is to understand and optimize the underlying processes in order to increase efficiency when dealing with multiple tasks at the same time. Growing demands on information processing due to increasing multimedia interactions call for higher efficiency. Does the human mind have the structural and functional means for efficient multitasking? The architectural properties of the nervous system allow for widely distributed simultaneous neural processing in billions of neurons. However in multitasking, severe performance costs occur when combining even the simplest cognitive tasks for simultaneous execution. Based on the current literature, we answer two key questions of multitasking: (1) What constitutes efficient and adaptive multitasking? (2) Which processes determine multitasking efficiency?

Most researchers explain typical performance decrements in multitasking with a structural capacity limitation, a so-called processing bottleneck (e.g., Pashler, 1998), at which certain cognitive processes proceed serially (i.e., one at a time only). Such a conceptualization of multitasking excludes the possibility of parallel (i.e., simultaneous) cognitive processing of multiple task components1. Others argue that parallel processing is generally possible (e.g., by means of capacity sharing, ), but that serial processing reflects the more efficient and thus primarily pursued multitasking strategy (e.g., ; ). In this review we will outline this debate, specifically focusing on the characteristics of serial versus parallel processing options and how they are theoretically conceptualized as well as empirically assessed in multitasking. For this, we restrict this review on multitasking situations, in which two speeded choice-reaction tasks have to be performed at the same time, i.e., dual tasks (for reviews of sequential multitasking, i.e., task switching, see ; ; ). We will further elaborate on the question of why people pursue the common aspiration of doing more than one task at a time (engaging in parallel processing) with the intention of increasing their performance efficiency, while serial processing is in fact more efficient. To provide a more complete picture on the debate of parallel versus serial processing, we will also discuss under which circumstances adopting a more parallel processing strategy represents the favorable multitasking solution.

Capabilities and Limitations of the Cognitive System for Multitasking

Serial Processing Due to Limited Resources

Serial Processing Brain

Performing two or more tasks at the same time typically results in severe performance costs in terms of increased response latencies and/or error rates (Welford, 1952; Kahneman, 1973; ). On a theoretical level, these dual-task costs have often been explained by means of a structural capacity limitation in cognitive processing. Early work on multitasking, framed within the information processing theory, assumed that access to this single processing channel is scheduled sequentially, one task at a time. For example, when a first task (T1) enters the capacity-limited processing stage, processing of an additional task (T2) is put to a halt until T1 critical stage processing is finished (see Figure Figure1).1). Following this logic, serial task scheduling is the consequence of a capacity-limited processing bottleneck that is structural in nature (Welford, 1952; Broadbent, 1958). This view of a structural limitation and a passive bottleneck-scheduling process is the core assumption of the influential and to date still widely accepted response-selection bottleneck (RSB) model (Pashler and Johnston, 1989; ). Following the stage logic of cognitive processing (Sternberg, 1969), peripheral processing stages of two tasks (e.g., perception, motor response) proceed in parallel. Capacity limitation arises at central processing stages (e.g., response selection) that do not proceed at the same time (see Figure Figure1A,1A, ; Pashler and Johnston, 1989)2. This view of a structural capacity limitation for central processing stages is still prevalent in human cognitive neuroscience and textbook psychology, most likely due to the observation that even the simplest and/or highly trained cognitive operations are subject to substantial processing limitations when combined with another task (e.g., ). However, there is less consensus about whether this processing bottleneck reflects a structural (Pashler, 1998) or a strategic () limitation.

Serial Processing Psychology

Schematic illustration of serial task processing (A) and different forms of parallel processing (B,C) of two tasks in the framework of an assumed capacity-limited central processing stage. Dashed lines illustrate the changes in result patterns when assuming different forms of parallel processing. Note that although theoretical models are explained in terms of response time (RT) pattern, the same logic also applies to error rates. (A) Illustration of the response-selection bottleneck (RSB) model as explanation for severe dual-task processing limitations (). Each task consists of different processing stages (i.e., P, perception; RS, response selection; MR, motor response). Processing in some stages can occur in parallel (in white). Processing of other critical stages cannot occur simultaneously (shaded), because they rely on the same capacity-limited processing channel. When both tasks overlap substantially (e.g., short stimulus onset asynchrony, SOA), Task 2 (T2) processing is interrupted, because RS2 processing has to wait until RS1 processing is completed (psychological refractory period, PRP). At long SOA, no interruption occurs, as critical stages do not overlap. This results in the typical pattern of performance decrements in T2 at short SOA (high dual-task load) compared to long SOA (low dual-task load). Task 1 (T1) processing is only little affected by temporal task overlap. (B) Crosstalk refers to the observation that T2 processing impacts on T1 processing, which has been taken as evidence for parallel processing despite an assumed RSB. Crosstalk effects are typically measured in response latency in T1 (RT1). The impact of T2 processing on central stage processing in T1 can be both beneficial or costly with decreasing or increasing RT1, respectively (e.g., Koch and Prinz, 2002). Importantly, any influence of T2 processing on T1, shortening or prolonging RT1, will back-propagate onto T2 (; ; ). Changes in RT1 due to crosstalk should thus also be obtainable in response latency in T2 (RT2). Theoretically, crosstalk effects are not compatible with classical models of a single-channel theory (e.g., RSB model) and favor explanations in terms of capacity sharing (see C). However, assumptions of serial processing according to the RSB model can be preserved when assuming that different sub-components of RS2 can operate in parallel. Some authors thus distinguish response activation (RA) processes from more classical response-selection processes as the basis for interacting central components between two tasks (; ; ). (C) Capacity models assume that the central bottleneck is not immutable but flexible. The processing limitation arises, because two central processes require access to the same cognitive resources. Available resources are divided between the two tasks for the period during which both central stages overlap. The allocation of resources to the tasks at hand depends on task factors (e.g., instruction, incentives). Extreme forms can mimic a central bottleneck, with 100% resources allocated to T1 and 0% to T2. The more resources are shared between the two tasks (e.g., 70/30 or 50/50), the higher the RT1 increase and RT2 decrease at short SOA. This resource allocation is assumed to be realized by mechanisms of cognitive control (for details, see text).

This theoretical assumption that central-processing limitations cause dual-task costs has largely been derived from experimental paradigms in which two choice reaction tasks are presented with varying temporal intervals (i.e., stimulus onset asynchronies, SOAs) between the stimulus of T1 (S1) and the stimulus of T2 (S2; e.g., ; Pashler and Johnston, 1989; Schubert, 1999). The temporal task overlap (and thus dual-task load) can be experimentally manipulated to examine dual-task costs and thus to assess the microstructure of dual-task interference (Pashler and Johnston, 1989; McCann and Johnston, 1992; ; see , 1998; , for reviews).

While performance in Task 1 has been assumed to be unaffected by the manipulation of temporal task overlap, performance in Task 2 has been hypothesized to critically depend on the temporal proximity of both tasks (Figure (Figure1A).1A). The larger the temporal overlap between both tasks (the shorter the SOA between S1 and S2), the slower the responses and higher the error rates in Task 2. The difference in response time in Task 2 (RT2) between short and long SOA has been called the psychological refractory period (PRP) effect (; ). The PRP effect reflects a widely used measure of dual-task costs evolving at the capacity-limited response-selection stage in dual-tasking (Figure (Figure1A).1A). Importantly, these dual-task costs (i.e., performance increments at high temporal task overlap compared to low temporal task overlap) serve as marker for multitasking efficiency.

Demonstrating Parallel Processing Under the Assumption of Limited Resources

Within the PRP paradigm, the assumption of parallel processing means that central cognitive processing in T2 can proceed in parallel to central capacity-limited stage processing in T1 (Figures (Figures1B1B,,C).C). Structural bottleneck models deny the possibility of central T2 processing during the bottleneck (i.e., the PRP). Yet, parallel T2 processing during the T1 bottleneck stage has been demonstrated with two different approaches, yielding to two distinct result patterns, i.e., the locus of slack logic and the backward crosstalk logic.

In the locus of slack logic (Pashler and Johnston, 1989; McCann and Johnston, 1992), the possibility of parallel processing is assessed in performance measures of T2. For this, the duration (e.g., difficulty) of a central cognitive process in T2 is manipulated. The relationship between easy and difficult conditions and SOA is particularly important in these cases. If no difficulty differences are obtained at short SOA, an absorption into the bottleneck (and hence parallel processing during the bottleneck) is assumed. If the manipulated central process cannot proceed during the bottleneck, the duration/difficulty manipulation should reveal additivity with the SOA manipulation. The manipulation should be visible to the same extent at short and long SOA (for successful demonstrations of parallel central processing using locus of slack, see ; ; ; ).

Backward crosstalk logic is based on the empirical finding that processing of two tasks rarely occurs independently (). Especially in conditions of high temporal proximity (i.e., short SOA) and high task similarity (i.e., both tasks share dimensional overlap), the likelihood of between-task interaction is increased. Figure Figure1B1B illustrates the possibility of this crosstalk. Central T2 task processing affects central T1 processing prior to completion of Task 1 bottleneck-stage processing (; , ; ; ; Koch and Prinz, 2002; ; ). As a consequence, any T1 processes subjected to the central bottleneck are either facilitated or prolonged. In contrast to the locus of slack logic, evidence for parallel processing based on crosstalk logic therefore comes from modulations of performance measures in T1 caused by central T2 processing.

A seminal study using this logic was provided by . In his experiment, participants responded to the color (red/green) of letters in T1 and to their identity (H/S) in T2. Crosstalk effects on RT1 were demonstrated when response codes for both tasks overlapped. More specifically, manual T1 responses to letter colors (e.g., red-left and green-right response) and verbal T2 responses to letter identity (e.g., saying left to an H and right to an S) produced substantial crosstalk interference (RT1 prolongation), when the verbal answer right coincided with the left manual response (response-category mismatch). T2 processing facilitated T1 response activation (RT1 decrease), when the verbal answer right was accompanied with the right manual response (response-category match). S-R translation processes of T1 and T2 were not serial and discrete but proceeded in parallel, when dimensional overlap between tasks was provided (see also ; Koch and Prinz, 2002; ; ; ; ). This finding of significant central T2 processing during the PRP challenges single-channel theories of serial processing (). Instead, the observed result pattern is in line with a capacity sharing assumption. The finding of crosstalk can however be reconciled with the notion of the RSB model, when adding a distinction between response activation (RA) and response identification (RI). While the former features no capacity limitations, thus allowing for crosstalk, the latter requires all central resources, causing a bottleneck (; ; ).

Further evidence for parallel central processing in dual tasks comes from electrophysiological studies. Behavioral crosstalk effects, as reported in , are accompanied by deflections in the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) in T2, reflecting T2 RA processes that start prior to completion of central bottleneck processing in T1 (). Similarly, measuring the LRP during T1 processing, showed that the time between S1 onset and onset of the stimulus-locked LRP was modulated by central T2 processing, thus providing further evidence for parallel central T2 processing.

Parallel Processing Without the Assumption of Limited Resources

The strict notion of limited resources and the consequential serial processing has been questioned on both experimental and theoretical grounds. It can be traced back to early attentional-filter conceptions of and the assumption of multiple specialized resources (Norman and Bobrow, 1975; Wickens, 1984; but see Allport, 1980).

Early work in the 1970s identified factors such as task similarity and task practice that crucially determine the possibility of parallel task processing. For example, argued that dual-task costs do not result from exceeding the capacity of a single-channel processor but from the difficulty of separating two similar tasks. When combining two highly dissimilar tasks (e.g., repeating continuous speech and sight-reading difficult piano music), piano players were able to demonstrate parallel processing of both tasks with a quality comparable to single-task processing (; see also Shaffer, 1975; Wickens, 1984). Similarly, Shaffer (1975) showed that participants skilled in typewriting can easily perform copy-typing (typing from a sheet) with a verbal shadowing task in parallel, but fail in combining an audio-typing task with reading from a sheet (for more recent research on the role of modality pairings when determining dual-task costs, see ; ; ; ; ). Although the aforementioned early studies revealed impressive capabilities in multitasking, many of these studies have been criticized, because their task timing allowed for fast switches between task-component processing (e.g., multiplexing). It is thus conceivable that the results only mimic parallel processing instead of actually representing it.

Another demonstration of parallel processing was achieved by administering a large amount of dual-task practice. Spelke et al. (1976) trained two participants to read short stories, while writing lists of words at dictation. Training took part over a period of 17 weeks (with five 1-h sessions per week). After the training, dual-task performance approached the quality of individual single-task performance. Because people easily develop skills through practice, the authors postulated that the potential of skill acquisition in any domain might question the general concept of limited cognitive capacity. In 2001, the assumption of parallel processing by extensive practice received a revival, mainly due to the implementation of timing- and interference-controlled dual-task paradigms. In an influential study, presented two choice reaction tasks (e.g., a visual-manual and an auditory-vocal task) simultaneously (SOA = 0 ms) and with equal task priority. Multitasking efficiency was captured in the extent to which performing each task in a dual-task context equals single-task performance. The criterion of perfect time sharing (dual-task performance equals single-task performance) was achieved after only five practice sessions (see also ; ; ; Liepelt et al., 2011a,; ). Proponents of the RSB model however argued that practice leads to shortening of processing stages which, in turn, reduces the effects of the bottleneck instead of bypassing it (e.g., latent bottleneck assumptions, see, e.g., ; ).

Theoretical Models Allowing for Parallel Processing

Resource models represent a major group of theoretical models that allow for the possibility of parallel task processing. In line with single-channel theories, they also assume a strict capacity limitation in central cognitive processing. In contrast to the RSB model however, they incorporate the idea that the available somewhat limited resources can be scheduled and allocated to specific task processing (Kahneman, 1973; Norman and Bobrow, 1975). An extension of the assumption of an unspecific central resource (Kahneman, 1973) is the idea of multiple specific resources (Wickens, 1984, 2002). Under the assumption of multiple resources, parallel processing can occur, for example, if task components are scheduled in different processing threads that are coordinated and scheduled by different resources (e.g., ).

According to resource models, dual-task costs arise, because processing of different task components requires the same limited resources (Kahneman, 1973; ; ). In these cases, resources are allocated in an all or none fashion. The classical PRP effect would reflect an extreme form of resource sharing of 100% of resources to T1 and 0% to T2, mimicking a capacity-limited central processing bottleneck due to the instruction of T1 priority. In contrast to RSB models, limited processing resources can however be shared between two tasks in varying proportions (e.g., 80% T1 and 20% T2). The more resources are shared, the more parallel processing occurs. With this logic, capacity-sharing models incorporate the central bottleneck assumption (; , ; ) and account for often observed findings of between-task crosstalk in PRP-like paradigms (; ; Koch and Prinz, 2002; ; ; ; ).

The proposition of a flexible allocation of resources requires a definition of how attentional resources are distributed. Arguments have been made that the allocation of attentional resources is not incidental but depends on instructions (; ), task priority, and outcome value (). It reflects strategic resource scheduling, typically realized by cognitive control processes (; ; ). The assumption of strategic and flexible allocation of processing capacity is supported by recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) findings indicating that brain areas associated with cognitive control are activated during dual-tasking (e.g., ; ; ; ; ). In addition, a recent fMRI study adopting a dual-task paradigm introduced by to create conditions of more serial versus more parallel task processing (for details, see below) provided evidence for distinct neuroanatomical correlates of response selection depending on task-processing constraints. In more detail, under conditions of increased parallel task processing response selection mechanisms operated at the striatal level, whereas under conditions of increased serial task processing response selection was accompanied by primarily lateral prefrontal cortex activation ().

Theories that postulate the involvement of cognitive control functions to explain limitations in multitasking do not necessarily feature assumptions of general limited resources. Multitasking limitations occur due to competing processes that require access to the same local and task-specific resources (Navon and Gopher, 1979). This notion is important, because instead of indicating structural constraints of limited capacity, the observed limitations reflect functional constraints (e.g., computational limitation). Functional constraints arise, when the same representation is used “for different purposes by multiple processes” (, p. 130). For example, in cognitive control theories of dual tasking such as the executive control of visual attention (ECTVA) theory (), the observation of serial task processing results from the effort to avoid interactions between multiple task-component processing (e.g., between stimulus-response bindings). This has been formulated in terms of the dual-task binding problem that describes the challenge to correctly map stimuli of each task (S1 and S2) onto the correct responses in each task (R1 and R2 for Task 1 and Task 2, respectively). To solve the binding problem and therefore to minimize the risk of response reversals and/or confusion, a strategy of serial processing is adopted (). The cognitive system can flexibly respond to requirements of the dual-task situation () by adopting a sequential processing strategy, if the risk of task reversals is high. As a consequence, serial processing of two tasks appears to be the more efficient processing strategy in general (; ) with the functionality of reducing between-task interference (; ; ; ; ). Choosing a serial processing mode thus reflects a functional and strategic option (see also ).

Why Do Individuals Employ a Parallel Task-Processing Mode in the Face of Increased Between-Task Interference?

We have established that individuals are able to adopt a strategy of parallel task-component processing when performing more than one task at a time. Given that parallel processing is associated with increased risks of between-task interference and is less efficient, why should participants opt for this processing strategy in the first place? provided a first answer to this question by showing that parallel processing has the means to outperform serial processing in terms of dual-task efficiency. They defined dual-task efficiency as the total time that it takes to complete two tasks (RT1 + RT2 = total reaction time, TRT). In two experiments, the PRP logic with two independent tasks was applied and the ratio of short to long SOA was varied in a list-wide proportion-SOA manipulation, i.e., participants performed lists with mostly short SOAs and lists with mostly long SOAs. The authors not only showed that lists with mostly short SOAs produced typical result patterns of parallel processing (i.e., increased RT1 and decreased RT2, Figure Figure1C)1C) but also revealed that list-wide SOA manipulations determined the efficiency of parallel and serial processing modes. Importantly, in lists with mostly short SOAs parallel processing turned out to be more efficient than serial processing. In typical PRP paradigms the temporal proximity between tasks (SOAs) is rather balanced. Thus, a serial processing strategy would seem favorable there.

Another reason for the fact that we mostly observe serial processing when testing participants with typical PRP-like dual-task paradigms in our laboratories is the strong T1 priority instruction. Strong emphasis on T1 performance favors a resource allocation that primarily or even entirely benefits T1 processing (e.g., 100% of available resources are allocated onto T1). Accordingly, result patterns that speak for serial processing are not surprising. Yet, when specific priority instructions are lifted, participants freely choose a moderate parallel processing strategy (), resulting in increased crosstalk between tasks (Figure (Figure1B).1B). An explanation of why participants may adopt a less efficient processing mode was put forward by , showing that parallel processing is associated with less mental effort, as confirmed by reduced levels of peripheral physiological measures and subjective effort ratings in parallel compared to more serial processing. Therefore, although parallel processing is not the most efficient way of dual-task processing (in most cases), it seems to reflect a less effortful processing strategy compared to strict serial processing. Given the choice, participants seem to adopt the processing mode of least mental effort (Hull, 1943; ).

Shifting Between Parallel and Serial Processing Modes as Marker of Adaptive Behavior

Even though the question of whether and to what extent parallel task processing is possible in dual-task performance is frequently discussed in the literature (), it might not be what we cognitive psychologists need to ask ourselves in this context. Instead, in our opinion, two key questions to guide current discussions and future research efforts result from the work as summarized above: (1) How is the shift between these complementary dual-task processing modes realized? (2) Under which conditions are parallel and serial processing the more adaptive and thus favorable choice? As a reply to the first question, we propose that optimized and efficient multitasking reflects the ability to flexibly adopt an either parallel or serial task-processing mode, depending on situational demands. Multitasking requires an individual to maintain a balance between two antagonistic types of performance optimization, i.e., minimizing between-task interference (by increasing serial task processing) and minimizing mental effort (by allowing for more parallel processing). Flexibly adjusting the degree of more serial versus more parallel dual-task processing to changing task and context requirements reflects high levels of adaptability in dynamic environments including the online analysis of contextual features, their translation into performance requirements, implementation of the optimal processing mode, and continuous monitoring of both the environmental demands and performance outcome as basis for further tuning, where required (Goschke, 2013; ).

Raising the question as to whether participants can top-down control their multitasking processing mode, recent studies provide evidence that individuals are able to implement substantial block-by-block switches between more parallel and more serial task-processing modes while continuing to perform the same task when instructed to do so (). In the framework of ECTVA (), instructions are assumed to define a set of cognitive control parameters (e.g., task priority, attentional breath, etc.) which, applied to a dual-task context, means that they determine the degree of serial versus parallel processing. On the cognitive process level, the extent of serial task processing might be then translated into two aspects, i.e., (a) the prioritization of Task 1 processing () and (b) a temporary inhibition of additional Task 2 processing ().

In addition to the top-down regulation, there is evidence for context-driven shifts between more parallel versus more serial dual-task processing. For example, the aforementioned expectation of the temporal overlap between two tasks () and task difficulty determine the degree of parallel versus serial task processing (e.g., task difficulty induces a bias toward increased serial dual-task processing which is reflected in steeper RT2 declines with increasing SOA; ; ). Recently, we also provided evidence for a complete bottom-up adjustment of parallel versus serial processing (). Using a crosstalk approach (Figure (Figure1B),1B), we tested whether participants are able to extract statistical contingencies (i.e., the probability of between-task interference) from the task context and use this information to adjust the amount of parallel versus serial task processing. More specifically, dual-task conditions with high risks of crosstalk interference (80% crosstalk interference trials) and dual-task conditions with low risks of crosstalk interference (20% crosstalk interference trials) were presented at distinguished locations on the screen. Although overall likelihood of crosstalk interference was 50/50, participants were able to adjust the strength of task shielding (by means of increased serial processing) in a location-specific manner. The extent of serial processing was significantly increased for the location with high likelihood of crosstalk interference, which resulted in reduced crosstalk for trials at that location. In trials presented at the location with low interference likelihood, shielding was reduced and higher levels of parallel processing were observed. Therefore, if a context requires more protection of T1 processing from T2 influences, task shielding is increased, resulting in less parallel processing.

Studying impacting factors of additional task performance on prioritized motor movements, we demonstrated that the preceding trial history (i.e., conflict between tasks in the previous trial) influences the balance between parallel and serial task processing (). Using continuous motor-execution tasks (i.e., mouse movements in both tasks), we showed that large amounts of crosstalk interference during the previous trial (N-1) resulted in the adoption of a more serial task-processing mode, as evidenced by reduced crosstalk interference in Trial N. Such sequential modulations are typically accounted for by response conflicts triggering the recruitment of cognitive control (; ). In the dual-task context, this reflects a bias toward increased serial task processing to reduce crosstalk interference.

We further demonstrated that the balance between complementary dual-task processing modes is determined by internal situational features such as variations in an individual’s mood, task-preceding cognitive control state, or acute stress levels. First, we administered a mood-induction procedure (with controlled arousal effects) prior to a dual task with crosstalk. Participants that underwent a negative mood-induction protocol displayed higher levels of serial task processing (e.g., less between-task interference) than participants that underwent positive mood induction (). Second, we manipulated the level of parallel versus serial processing by activating different cognitive control states prior to dual-task performance (). Participants solved different types of creativity tasks associated with either cognitive flexibility (i.e., divergent thinking) or cognitive persistence (i.e., convergent thinking) prior to the dual-task session. In the convergent-thinking group, participants performed an adapted version of the remote association test (RAT; ), in the divergent-thinking group, participants performed a version of the alternative uses task (AUT; Guilford, 1967) in order to induce convergent versus divergent thinking, respectively. The convergent-thinking group displayed stronger serial processing to reduce between-task interference compared to both divergent-thinking and control group. Third, we investigated the effect of acute stress on dual-task processing modes. Following an acute psychosocial stressor (Trier Social Stress Test), participants adopted a more resource-saving processing strategy of increased parallel processing compared to controls without the stress experience (). Stressed participants allowed for increased levels of between-task interference, presumably because a strict sequential and serial scheduling of task processing would be too effortful and resource-demanding. We interpreted our finding as a sacrificing and compensatory strategy in order to maintain overall high dual-task performance. The notion that parallel processing reflects a more resource-saving processing mode and is mentally less demanding () fits nicely with our observation of compensatory cognitive strategy changes as a result of acute stress experience. Using a different dual-task paradigm consisting of two independent tasks without the possibility of between-task interactions, stress-related compensatory strategy shifts entailed an increased T1 priority focus, speeding T1 component processing and, as a result, reducing critical bottleneck stage processing ().

These examples illustrate that the adoption of a more parallel or a more serial task-processing mode largely depends on the conditions under which multiple tasks are performed. With this, the situation-dependent implementation of complementary task-processing modes in multitasking can be integrated into a general framework posting the regulation of cognitive control as fundamental basis underlying adaptive goal-directed behavior (Cohen et al., 2004, ; Goschke, 2013). Adaptive action control requires the dynamic adjustment between complementary control demands. While task-irrelevant stimuli need to be ignored and blocked from being processed in order to prevent interference with task-relevant processing (goal shielding), complete shielding is dysfunctional and even potentially harmful, as it would prevent the individual from monitoring the environment for potentially relevant stimuli (background monitoring) that may signal a change in action goal (e.g., stimuli that imply danger). Instead, a dynamic regulation of these complementary control processes is required, reflecting a tradeoff between antagonistic constraints (see Goschke, 2003, 2013; , for an overview). Such “control dilemmas” describe the need to be continuously and flexibly adjusted based on (changing) environmental demands (Goschke, 2003; Cohen et al., 2004, ). A recent argument is that dysfunctional control parameter settings in terms of sustained biases toward one particular (often extreme) control state might represent an endophenotype of a variety of mental disorders (e.g., ).

Such extreme biases and their behavioral consequences have been primarily investigated in healthy populations. For example, an experimentally induced bias toward high stability comes at the cost of reduced cognitive flexibility (; ; ), which, if maintained, might not only turn out to be dysfunctional in situations calling for cognitive flexibility but additionally manifest in overall cognitive rigidity and compulsive behavior (). A key future scientific endeavor will be to determine the cognitive mechanisms that underlie the flexible adoption of complementary task-processing modes in general.

Applying this framework, multitasking in itself constitutes a prime control dilemma, in which a crosstalk-avoiding serial processing strategy counters an effort-saving parallel processing strategy. Complete T1 shielding and thus T2 blocking is dysfunctional, as successful multitasking requires the attendance and processing of T2 components. Yet, the efficiency of adaptive multitasking might be seen in the flexible selection of a situation-adequate processing strategy within a continuum from serial to parallel task processing. Such a conception also has direct consequences for defining efficient multitasking, and it raises the question of how to optimize the flexible and context-sensitive adoption of complementary task-processing modes.

We would like to conclude this discussion about parallel and serial processing in multitasking by emphasizing that the inclusion of closely related topics into further investigations of multitasking will be crucial as additional source to inform our understanding of efficient multitasking and its underlying cognitive processes. First of all, incorporating an individual difference perspective on multitasking ability in general and the adaptive adjustment of task-processing strategies in particular might hold promise for gaining novel insights into factors of optimized multitasking performance. For example, individuals frequently engaging in multitasking are not necessarily the ones displaying efficient multitasking performance (; ). In fact, multitasking activity correlated negatively with multitasking ability but positively with impulsiveness and sensation seeking (). Individuals that frequently multitask (e.g., heavy media multitaskers) are more easily distracted by irrelevant information and less able to focus on a single goal (). In the context of the outlined discussion, this behavior can be framed as a task-processing bias continuously tuned toward heightened levels of parallel processing. Another promising finding in individual differences in multitasking abilities that warrants further investigation is the detection of “supertaskers,” i.e., individuals who do not show performance decrements in multitasking compared to single-task performance (). Their ability has been explained in terms of a more efficient recruitment of cognitive control and an increased ability to maintain and coordinate multiple goals and limitations in information processing, enabling supertaskers to more effectively deal with situations of heightened cognitive load ().

Secondly, research contrasting tentative processes underlying multitasking with analogous processes in related fields of the study of attentional limitations, e.g., to more clearly determine at which points more serial and more parallel task processing are more adaptive and efficient (see, for example ). To illustrate this point, unified bottleneck theories propose close similarities between attentional limitations in sensory consolidation as in the attentional blink () and attentional limitations in response selection as in the PRP paradigm (; ; ). Yet, while PRP research often aims at optimizing dual-task performance by increasing the engagement of cognitive control (e.g., through an emphasis on speed plus reward), it has been shown that limitations in the attentional blink arise from an overinvestment of cognitive control (; ). Therefore, future research is needed to determine whether strategies of increasing versus decreasing the engagement of cognitive control might be promising when aiming at reducing dual-task costs in standard training protocols.

Thirdly, an important question to guide future research in this area is in which respect the ability to flexibly adopt complementary task-processing modes is related to fluid intelligence, further specifying the currently investigated link between core executive control competencies and fluid intelligence as well as flexible adaptation to environmental changes as a hallmark of fluid intelligence (; ; ; ; ).

Conclusion

Starting from the question of what constitutes adaptive multitasking performance, we reviewed empirical evidence for two processing modes in multitasking that are not mutually exclusive, i.e., serial versus parallel task-component processing. Demonstrating that parallel task processing is indeed possible when performing more than one task at a time has challenged the view that the frequently observed multitasking costs represent an inevitable consequence of a structural capacity limitation. Instead, it suggests that these multitasking costs may signal a functional limitation (e.g., with the purpose of avoiding crosstalk). In the second part of the review, we highlighted that evidence for parallel processing critically depends on the theoretical and methodological basis under which multitasking performance is assessed.

While serial task processing appears to be the most efficient multitasking processing strategy, participants are able to adopt parallel processing. Moreover, parallel processing can even outperform serial processing under certain conditions. Based on these highlighted insights into multitasking performance, future research aiming to further understand the nature of parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks to unveil the secrets of multitasking efficiency needs to take into account the preconditions and environmental constraints under which multitasking is performed. We believe that a flexible and context-sensitive recruitment of a more serial or more parallel processing strategy enables the agent to flexibly adjust to environmental demands, providing important mechanisms for adaptive intelligent behavior (Cohen et al., 2004, ; Goschke, 2013).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This study was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG, SFB 940/1, Projects A3 to RF and B5 to FP).

Footnotes

1In more detail, serial processing means strict sequential processing of certain critical (e.g., capacity-demanding) cognitive processes due to the assumed bottleneck. When a cognitive process A is underway, another cognitive process B cannot operate at the same time. For the assumption of parallel processing, however, processes A and B proceed simultaneously (at least to certain degrees), i.e., in parallel to each other and despite the capacity limitation of the bottleneck.

2It should be noted that there are alternative conceptions of the bottleneck theory, that mainly differ in the location of the capacity-limited processing stages, proposing either motor execution bottlenecks (e.g., Keele, 1973; ; ) or multiple bottlenecks in the information-processing stream ().

References

  • Allport A. (1980). “Attention and performance,” in Cognitive Psychology. New Directions, ed. Claxton G., editor. (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul; ), 112–153. [Google Scholar]
  • Allport A., Antonis B., Reynolds P. (1972). On the division of attention: a disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.24, 225–235. 10.1080/00335557243000102 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Au J., Sheehan E., Tsai N., Duncan G. J., Buschkuehl M., Jaeggi S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev.22, 366–377. 10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Beste C., Yildiz A., Meissner T. W., Wolf O. T. (2013). Stress improves task processing efficiency in dual-tasks. Behav. Brain Res.252, 260–265. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.06.013 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Botvinick M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev.108, 624–652. 10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.624 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bratzke D., Rolke B., Ulrich R. (2009). The source of execution-related dual-task interference: motor bottleneck or response monitoring?J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.35, 1413–1426. 10.1037/a0015874 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Broadbent D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. London: Pergamon Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen J. D., Aston-Jones G., Gilzenrat M. S. (2004). “A system-level perspective on attention and cognitive control: guided activation, adaptive gating, conflict monitoring, and exploitation vs. exploration,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, ed. Posner M. I., editor. (New York, NY: Guilford Press; ), 71–90. [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen J. D., McClure S. M., Yu A. J. (2007). Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.362, 933–942. 10.1098/rstb.2007.2098 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • De Jong R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.19, 965–980. 10.1037/0096-1523.19.5.965 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Deutsch J. A., Deutsch D. (1963). Attention—some theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rev.70, 80–90. 10.1037/H0039515 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Diamond A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol.64, 135–168. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dreisbach G., Goschke T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.30, 343–353. 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.343 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Duncan J. (1979). Divided attention: the whole is more than the sum of its parts. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.5, 216–228. 10.1037/0096-1523.5.2.216 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Duncan J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends Cogn. Sci.14, 172–179. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Duncan J., Parr A., Woolgar A., Thompson R., Bright P., Cox S., et al. (2008). Goal neglect and Spearman’s g: competing parts of a complex task. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.137, 131–148. 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.131 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dux P. E., Ivanoff J., Asplund C. L., Marois R. (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron52, 1109–1120. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dux P. E., Tombu M. N., Harrison S., Rogers B. P., Tong F., Marois R. (2009). Training improves multitasking performance by increasing the speed of information processing in human prefrontal cortex. Neuron63, 127–138. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.005 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Feng S. F., Schwemmer M., Gershman S. J., Cohen J. D. (2014). Multitasking versus multiplexing: toward a normative account of limitations in the simultaneous execution of control-demanding behaviors. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.14, 129–146. 10.3758/s13415-013-0236-9 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferreira V. S., Pashler H. (2002). Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.28, 1187–1199. 10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1187 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fischer R., Gottschalk C., Dreisbach G. (2014). Context-sensitive adjustment of cognitive control in dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.40, 399–416. 10.1037/a0034310 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fischer R., Hommel B. (2012). Deep thinking increases task-set shielding and reduces shifting flexibility in dual-task performance. Cognition123, 303–307. 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.015 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fischer R., Miller J., Schubert T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Mem. Cogn.35, 1685–1699. 10.3758/BF03193502 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fischer R., Schubert T. (2008). Valence processing bypassing the response selection bottleneck? Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Exp. Psychol.55, 203–211. 10.1027/1618-3169.55.3.203 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Garner K. G., Tombu M. N., Dux P. E. (2014). The influence of training on the attentional blink and psychological refractory period. Atten. Percept. Psychophys.76, 979–999. 10.3758/s13414-014-0638-y [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goschke T. (2003). “Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective,” in Voluntary Action. An Issue at the Interface of Nature and Culture, eds Maasen S., Prinz W., Roth G., editors. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ), 49–85. [Google Scholar]
  • Goschke T. (2013). “Volition in action: intentions, control dilemmas and the dynamic regulation of cognitive intentional control,” in Action Science: Foundations of an Emerging Discipline, eds Prinz W., Beisert A., Herwig A., editors. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ), 409–434. [Google Scholar]
  • Goschke T. (2014). Dysfunctions of decision-making and cognitive control as transdiagnostic mechanisms of mental disorders: advances, gaps, and needs in current research. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res.23(Suppl. 1), 41–57. 10.1002/mpr.1410 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goschke T., Bolte A. (2014). Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: the role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia62, 403–423. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.015 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gratton G., Coles M. G., Donchin E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.121, 480–506. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Guilford J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  • Halvorson K. M., Hazeltine E. (2015). Do small dual-task costs reflect ideomotor compatibility or the absence of crosstalk. Psychon. Bull. Rev.10.3758/s13423-015-0813-8[Epub ahead of print]. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Han S. W., Marois R. (2013). The source of dual-task limitations: serial or parallel processing of multiple response selections?Attent. Percept. Psychophys.75, 1395–1405. 10.3758/s13414-013-0513-2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hazeltine E., Ruthruff E. (2006). Modality pairing effects and the response selection bottleneck. Psychol. Res.70, 504–513. 10.1007/s00426-005-0017-3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hazeltine E., Ruthruff E., Remington R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cogn. Psychol.52, 291–345. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.11.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hazeltine E., Teague D., Ivry R. B. (2002). Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.28, 527–545. 10.1037/0096-1523.28.3.527 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hazeltine E., Weinstein A., Ivry R. B. (2008). Parallel response selection after callosotomy. J. Cogn. Neurosci.20, 526–540. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20030 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hommel B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.24, 1368–1384. 10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Huestegge L., Koch I. (2013). Constraints in task-set control: modality dominance patterns among effector systems. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.142, 633–637. 10.1037/a0030156 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hull C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. New York, NY: Appleton-Century. [Google Scholar]
  • Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Jonides J., Perrig W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.105, 6829–6833. 10.1073/pnas.0801268105 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Janczyk M. (2013). Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.39, 1878–1887. 10.1037/a0033336 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Janczyk M., Pfister R., Hommel B., Kunde W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition132, 30–43. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kahneman D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • Keele S. W. (1973). Attention and Human Performance. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear. [Google Scholar]
  • Kiesel A., Steinhauser M., Wendt M., Falkenstein M., Jost K., Philipp A. M., et al. (2010). Control and interference in task switching–A review. Psychol. Bull.136, 849–874. 10.1037/a0019842 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Koch I. (2009). The role of crosstalk in dual-task performance: evidence from manipulating response-code overlap. Psychol. Res.73, 417–424. 10.1007/s00426-008-0152-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Koch I., Gade M., Schuch S., Philipp A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev.17, 1–14. 10.3758/PBR.17.1.1 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Koch I., Prinz W. (2002). Process interference and code overlap in dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.28, 192–201. 10.1037/0096-1523.28.1.192 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kool W., McGuire J. T., Rosen Z. B., Botvinick M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.139, 665–682. 10.1037/a0020198 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ko Y. T., Miller J. (2014). Locus of backward crosstalk effects on Task 1 in a psychological refractory period task. Exp. Psychol.61, 30–37. 10.1027/1618-3169/a000224 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lehle C., Hübner R. (2009). Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets. Psychol. Res.73, 707–726. 10.1007/s00426-008-0162-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lehle C., Steinhauser M., Hübner R. (2009). Serial or parallel processing in dual tasks: what is more effortful?Psychophysiology46, 502–509. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00806.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Levy J., Pashler H., Boer E. (2006). Central interference in driving: is there any stopping the psychological refractory period?Psychol. Sci.17, 228–235. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01690.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lien M. C., Proctor R. W. (2002). Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: implications for response selection. Psychon. Bull. Rev.9, 212–238. 10.3758/BF03196277 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lien M. C., Ruthruff E., Hsieh S., Yu Y. T. (2007). Parallel central processing between tasks: evidence from lateralized readiness potentials. Psychon. Bull. Rev.14, 133–141. 10.3758/BF03194040 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Liepelt R., Fischer R., Frensch P., Schubert T. (2011a). Practice-related reduction of dual-task costs under conditions of a manual-pedal response combination. J. Cogn. Psychol.23, 29–44. 10.1080/20445911.2011.448025 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Liepelt R., Strobach T., Frensch P., Schubert T. (2011b). Improved intertask coordination after extensive dual-task practice. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.64, 1251–1272. 10.1080/17470218.2010.543284 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Logan G. D., Gordon R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychol. Rev.108, 393–434. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Logan G. D., Schulkind M. D. (2000). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: i. Semantic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.26, 1072–1090. 10.1037/0096-1523.26.3.1072 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luria R., Meiran N. (2005). Increased control demand results in serial processing: evidence from dual-task performance. Psychol. Sci.16, 833–840. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01622.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Marois R., Ivanoff J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci.9, 296–305. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Marti S., Sigman M., Dehaene S. (2012). A shared cortical bottleneck underlying Attentional Blink and Psychological Refractory Period. Neuroimage59, 2883–2898. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.063 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McCann R. S., Johnston J. C. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.18, 471–484. 10.1037//0096-1523.18.2.471 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Medeiros-Ward N., Watson J. M., Strayer D. L. (2014). On supertaskers and the neural basis of efficient multitasking. Psychon. Bull. Rev.22, 876–883. 10.3758/s13423-014-0713-3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mednick S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychol. Rev.69, 220–232. 10.1037/h0048850 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Meiran N., Diamond G. M., Toder D., Nemets B. (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Res.185, 149–156. 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.044 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Meyer D. E., Kieras D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychol. Rev.104, 3–65. 10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychol. Res.70, 484–493. 10.1007/s00426-005-0011-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller J., Alderton M. (2006). Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.32, 149–165. 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.149 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller J., Reynolds A. (2003). The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.29, 1126–1142. 10.1037/0096-1523.29.6.1126 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller J., Ulrich R., Rolke B. (2009). On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies. Cogn. Psychol.58, 273–310. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.08.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Monsell S. (2003). Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci.7, 134–140. 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Navon D., Gopher D. (1979). On the economy of the human information-processing system. Psychol. Rev.86, 214–255. 10.1037/0033-295X.86.3.214 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Navon D., Miller J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.13, 435–448. 10.1037/0096-1523.13.3.435 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Navon D., Miller J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cogn. Psychol.44, 193–251. 10.1006/cogp.2001.0767 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Norman D. A., Bobrow D. G. (1975). Data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cogn. Psychol.7, 44–64. 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90004-3 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Oberauer K., Bialkova S. (2011). Serial and parallel processes in working memory after practice. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.37, 606–614. 10.1037/a0020986 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Oberauer K., Kliegl R. (2004). Simultaneous cognitive operations in working memory after dual-task practice. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.30, 689–707. 10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.689 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Olivers C. N., Nieuwenhuis S. (2006). The beneficial effects of additional task load, positive affect, and instruction on the attentional blink. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.32, 364–379. 10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.364 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ophir E., Nass C., Wagner A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.106, 15583–15587. 10.1073/pnas.0903620106 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Oriet C., Tombu M., Jolicoeur P. (2005). Symbolic distance affects two processing loci in the number comparison task. Mem. Cogn.33, 913–926. 10.3758/BF03193085 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pashler H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.10, 358–377. 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.358 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pashler H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol. Bull.116, 220–244. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pashler H. (1998). The Psychology of Attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Pashler H., Johnston J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.41A, 19–45. 10.1080/14640748908402351 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Plessow F., Fischer R., Kirschbaum C., Goschke T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. J. Cogn. Neurosci.23, 3218–3227. 10.1162/jocn_a_00024 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Plessow F., Schade S., Kirschbaum C., Fischer R. (2012). Better not to deal with two tasks at the same time when stressed? Acute psychosocial stress reduces task shielding in dual-task performance. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.12, 557–570. 10.3758/s13415-012-0098-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Raymond J. E., Shapiro K. L., Arnell K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink?J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.18, 849–860. 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ruthruff E., Johnston J. C., Van Selst M. (2001). Why practice reduces dual-task interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.27, 3–21. 10.1037//0096-1523.27.1.3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ruthruff E., Johnston J. C., Van Selst M., Whitsell S., Remington R. (2003). Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent?J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.29, 280–289. 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.280 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Salvucci D. D., Taatgen N. A. (2008). Threaded cognition: an integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychol. Rev.115, 101–130. 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.101 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sanbonmatsu D. M., Strayer D. L., Medeiros-Ward N., Watson J. M. (2013). Who multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. PLoS ONE8:e54402. 10.1371/journal.pone.0054402 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Scherbaum S., Gottschalk C., Dshemuchadse M., Fischer R. (2015). Action dynamics in multitasking: the impact of additional task factors on the execution of the prioritized motor movement. Front. Psychol.6:934. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00934 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schubert T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.25, 408–425. 10.1037/0096-1523.25.2.408 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schubert T., Fischer R., Stelzel C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.34, 376–397. 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.376 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schumacher E. H., Seymour T. L., Glass J. M., Fencsik D. E., Lauber E. J., Kieras D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychol. Sci.12, 101–108. 10.1111/1467-9280.00318 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Shaffer L. H. (1975). “Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks,” in Attention and Performance V, eds Rabbitt P. M. A., Dornic S., editors. (New York, NY: Academic Press; ), 157–167. [Google Scholar]
  • Sigman M., Dehaene S. (2006). Dynamics of the central bottleneck: dual-task and task uncertainty. PLoS Biol.4:e220. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040220 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Spelke E., Hirst W., Neisser U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition4, 215–230. [Google Scholar]
  • Stelzel C., Brandt S. A., Schubert T. (2009). Neural mechanisms of concurrent stimulus processing in dual tasks. Neuroimage48, 237–248. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.064 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Stelzel C., Schumacher E. H., Schubert T., D’Esposito M. (2005). The neural effect of stimulus-response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: an fMRI study. Psychol. Res.70, 514–525. 10.1007/s00426-005-0013-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sternberg S. (1969). “The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method,” in Attention and Performance II, ed. Koster W. G., editor. (Amsterdam, NL: North-Holland; ), 276–315. [Google Scholar]
  • Strobach T., Frensch P. A., Soutschek A., Schubert T. (2012). Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills. Psychol. Res. Psychol. Forsch.76, 794–811. 10.1007/s00426-011-0381-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Szameitat A. J., Schubert T., Müller K., Von Cramon D. Y. (2002). Localization of executive functions in dual-task performance with fMRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci.14, 1184–1199. 10.1162/089892902760807195 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Taatgen N. A., Juvina I., Schipper M., Borst J. P., Martens S. (2009). Too much control can hurt: a threaded cognition model of the attentional blink. Cogn. Psychol.59, 1–29. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.12.002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Tombu M., Asplund C. L., Dux P. E., Godwin D., Martin J. W., Marois R. (2011). A unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.108, 13426–13431. 10.1073/pnas.1103583108 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Tombu M., Jolicoeur P. (2002). All-or-none bottleneck versus capacity sharing accounts of the psychological refractory period phenomenon. Psychol. Res.66, 274–286. 10.1007/s00426-002-0101-x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Tombu M., Jolicoeur P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.29, 3–18. 10.1037//0096-1523.29.1.3 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ulrich R., Fernandez S. R., Jentzsch I., Rolke B., Schroter H., Leuthold H. (2006). Motor limitation in dual-task processing under ballistic movement conditions. Psychol. Sci.17, 788–793. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01783.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Watson J. M., Strayer D. L. (2010). Supertaskers: profiles in extraordinary multitasking ability. Psychon. Bull. Rev.17, 479–485. 10.3758/Pbr.17.4.479 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Welford A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—A review and a theory. Br. J. Psychol.43, 2–19. [Google Scholar]
  • Wickens C. D. (1984). “Processing resources in attention,” in Varieties of Attention, eds Parasuraman R., Beatty J., Davis R., editors. (New York, NY: Wiley; ), 63–101. [Google Scholar]
  • Wickens C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci.3, 159–177. 10.1080/14639220210123806 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wickens C. D., Goh J., Helleberg J., Horrey W. J., Talleur D. A. (2003). Attentional models of multitask pilot performance using advanced display technology. Hum. Factors45, 360–380. 10.1080/14639220210123806 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yildiz A., Beste C. (2014). Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking differentially involves mechanisms in the striatum and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct. Funct.10.1007/s00429-014-0847-0[Epub ahead of print]. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zwosta K., Hommel B., Goschke T., Fischer R. (2013). Mood states determine the degree of task shielding in dual-task performance. Cogn. Emot.27, 1142–1152. 10.1080/02699931.2013.772047 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA